• Original Paper
  • Published: 28 October 2022

The Homework Problems Checklist: Psychometric Properties and Usefulness in teens with and without ADHD

  • Marieke de Vries   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-2845-8956 1 ,
  • Saskia van der Oord   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-2771-0187 2 , 3 ,
  • Steven W. Evans   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-7283-2274 4 ,
  • George J. DuPaul   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-4601-3507 5 &
  • Bianca E. Boyer   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-2344-7334 6  

School Mental Health volume  15 ,  pages 260–271 ( 2023 ) Cite this article

448 Accesses

2 Citations

2 Altmetric

Metrics details

Homework problems are frequently encountered, especially among youth with ADHD. The Homework Problems Checklist (HPC) is a parent-rated questionnaire to assess homework problems that was studied with children but not extensively with teens. We assessed the psychometric properties of the HPC with teens with and without ADHD. Firstly, the factor structures from previous studies were fitted on a large representative Dutch teen community sample ( N  = 991; 10–18 years), and one Dutch ( N  = 118; 12–16 years) and two American ( N  = 348; 10–14 years, N  = 180; 13–17 years) samples with teens diagnosed with ADHD. Secondly, homework problems of the community sample and the samples of teens with ADHD were compared. Thirdly, the effects of age and sex on homework problems were tested. The results indicated that 1) a two-factor (1. Completion and 2. Management) model fitted the data adequately. Coefficients of congruence confirmed equal factor structures in the samples. 2) The samples with teens with ADHD had more homework problems than the community sample, and among those teens with ADHD the US samples had more homework problems than the Dutch sample. 3) In the community sample, older teens showed fewer homework completion problems than younger teens. Moreover, homework management problems decreased with age in females but not in males. Sex- and age effects for teens with ADHD were minimal. Raw and percentile scores of the community sample are provided. In sum, the HPC can be used to establish the severity and nature of homework problems in teens. In teens without ADHD, particularly females, homework problems decrease with age, but for teens with ADHD, homework problems persist during adolescence.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution .

Access options

Buy single article.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Rent this article via DeepDyve.

Note that we chose the terms ‘sex’, ‘male’ and ‘female’ because we only had information about participants’ biological sex, and not about their gender identity.

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fifth Edition. In Arlington. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.744053

Anesko, K. M., Schoiock, G., Ramirez, R., & Levine, F. M. (1987). The homework problem checklist: Assessing children’s homework difficulties. Behavioral Assessment.

Arbuckle, J. L. (2017). Amos (Version 25.0).

Arnold, L. E., Hodgkins, P., Kahle, J., Madhoo, M., & Kewley, G. (2020). Long-term outcomes of ADHD: Academic achievement and performance. Journal of Attention Disorders, 24 (1), 73–85.

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Bas, G., Senturk, C., & Cigerci, F. M. (2017). Homework and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Issues in Educational Research, 27 (1), 31.

Google Scholar  

Boyer, B. E., Geurts, H. M., Prins, P. J. M., & der OordVan, S. (2015). Two novel CBTs for adolescents with ADHD: the value of planning skills. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 24 (9), 1075–1090.

Article   Google Scholar  

Boyer, B. E., Geurts, H. M., der Oord, S., & Van. (2018). Planning skills of adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 22 (1), 46–57.

Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek. (2003). Schoolbeleving van de jeugd, 1997, 2001 en 2003. https://www.cbs.nl/nl-nl/cijfers/detail/70630ned?q=huiswerk

Cooper, H. (1989). Homework . Longman.

Book   Google Scholar  

Cooper, H., Robinson, J. C., & Patall, E. A. (2006). Does homework improve academic achievement? A synthesis of research, 1987–2003. Review of Educational Research, 76 (1), 1–62.

Crone, E. A. (2009). Executive functions in adolescence: Inferences from brain and behavior. Developmental Science, 12 (6), 825–830.

Danielson, M. L., Bitsko, R. H., Ghandour, R. M., Holbrook, J. R., Kogan, M. D., & Blumberg, S. J. (2018). Prevalence of parent-reported ADHD diagnosis and associated treatment among US children and adolescents, 2016. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 47 (2), 199–212.

DuPaul, G. J., Evans, S. W., Owens, J. S., Cleminshaw, C. L., Kipperman, K., Fu, Q., & Benson, K. (2021). School-based intervention for adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Effects on academic functioning. Journal of School Psychology , 87 , 48–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JSP.2021.07.001

DuPaul, G., Power, T., Anastopoulos, A., & Reid, R. (2016). ADHD rating scale-5 for children and adolescents: checklists, norms, and clinical interpretation.

Evans, S. W., Langberg, J. M., Schultz, B. K., Vaughn, A., Altaye, M., Marshall, S. A., & Zoromski, A. K. (2016). Evaluation of a school-based treatment program for young adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 84 (1), 15.

Field, A. (2018). Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics 5th ed.

Fleming, M., Fitton, C. A., Steiner, M. F. C., McLay, J. S., Clark, D., King, A., Mackay, D. F., & Pell, J. P. (2017). Educational and health outcomes of children treated for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA Pediatrics, 171 (7), e170691–e170691.

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Fletcher, J. M. (2014). The effects of childhood ADHD on adult labor market outcomes. Health Economics, 23 (2), 159–181.

Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Glutting, J. J., & Watkins, M. W. (2007). ADHD and achievement: Meta-analysis of the child, adolescent, and adult literatures and a concomitant study with college students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40 (1), 49–65. https://doi.org/10.1177/00222194070400010401

Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6 (1), 1–55.

Kantar TNS. (2016). Market research and market information group Kantar TNS the Netherlands, formerly known as TNS-NIPO.

Kent, K. M., Pelham, W. E., Molina, B. S., Sibley, M. H., Waschbusch, D. A., Yu, J., Gnagy, E. M., Biswas, A., Babinski, D. E., & Karch, K. M. (2011). The academic experience of male high school students with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39 (3), 451–462.

Kort, W., Schittekatte, M., Bosmans, M., Compaan, E., Dekker, P., Vermeir, G., & Verhaeghe, P. (2005). WISC-III NL : Handleiding en verantwoording.

Kyriazos, T. A. (2018). Applied Psychometrics: Sample Size and Sample Power Considerations in Factor Analysis (EFA, CFA) and SEM in General. Psychology, 9 , 2207–2230.

Lahey, B. B., & Willcutt, E. G. (2010). Predictive validity of a continuous alternative to nominal subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder for DSM–V. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 39 (6), 761–775.

Langberg, J. M., Arnold, L. E., Flowers, A. M., Altaye, M., Epstein, J. N., & Molina, B. S. G. (2010). Assessing homework problems in children with ADHD: Validation of a parent-report measure and evaluation of homework performance patterns. School Mental Health, 2 (1), 3–12.

Langberg, J. M., Becker, S. P., & Dvorsky, M. R. (2014). The association between sluggish cognitive tempo and academic functioning in youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42 (1), 91–103.

Langberg, J. M., Dvorsky, M. R., & Evans, S. W. (2013). What specific facets of executive function are associated with academic functioning in youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41 (7), 1145–1159.

Langberg, J. M., Dvorsky, M. R., Molitor, S. J., Bourchtein, E., Eddy, L. D., Smith, Z., Schultz, B. K., & Evans, S. W. (2016). Longitudinal evaluation of the importance of homework assignment completion for the academic performance of middle school students with ADHD. Journal of School Psychology, 55 , 27–38.

Langberg, J. M., Epstein, J. N., Becker, S. P., Girio-Herrera, E., & Vaughn, A. J. (2012). Evaluation of the homework, organization, and planning skills (HOPS) intervention for middle school students with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder as implemented by school mental health providers. School Psychology Review, 41 (3), 342–364. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.2012.12087514

Langberg, J. M., Epstein, J. N., Girio-Herrera, E., Becker, S. P., Vaughn, A. J., & Altaye, M. (2011). Materials organization, planning, and homework completion in middle-school students with ADHD: Impact on academic performance. School Mental Health, 3 (2), 93–101.

Lorenzo-Seva, U., & Ten Berge, J. M. F. (2006). Tucker’s congruence coefficient as a meaningful index of factor similarity. Methodology, 2 (2), 57–64.

Marchal, J. P., De Vries, M., Conijn, J., Rietman, A. B., Ijsselstijn, H., Tibboel, D., Haverman, L., Maurice-Stam, H., Oostrom, K. J., & Grootenhuis, M. A. (2019). Pediatric perceived cognitive functioning: Psychometric properties and normative data of the dutch item bank and short form. Cambridge. Org, 25 (8), 845–856. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719000572

Marx, I., Hacker, T., Yu, X., Cortese, S., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (2021). ADHD and the choice of small immediate over larger delayed rewards: A comparative meta-analysis of performance on simple choice-delay and temporal discounting. Journals Sagepub Com, 25 (2), 171–187. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054718772138

National Center for Education Statistics. (2016). Percentage of elementary and secondary school students who do homework, average time spent doing homework, percentage whose parents check that homework is done, and percentage whose parents help with homework, by frequency and selected characteristics: 2007, 2012, 2016, and 2019. In https://nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/d20/tables/dt20_227.40.asp?current=yes .

Patros, C. H. G., Alderson, R. M., Kasper, L. J., Tarle, S. J., Lea, S. E., & Hudec, K. L. (2016). Choice-impulsivity in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A meta-analytic review. In Clinical Psychology Review, 43 , 162–174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2015.11.001

Pelham, W. E., Evans, S. W., Gnagy, E. M., & Greenslade, K. E. (1992). Teacher ratings of Dsm-Iii-R symptoms for the disruptive behavior disorders - prevalence, factor-analyses, and conditional probabilities in a special-education sample. School Psychology Review, 21 (2), 285–299.

Power, T. J., Werba, B. E., Watkins, M. W., Angelucci, J. G., & Eiraldi, R. B. (2006). Patterns of parent-reported homework problems among ADHD-referred and non-referred children. School Psychology Quarterly, 21 (1), 13.

Ramos, A. A., Hamdan, A. C., & Machado, L. (2020). A meta-analysis on verbal working memory in children and adolescents with ADHD. In Clinical Neuropsychologist, 34 (5), 873–898. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2019.1604998

Sattler, J. (2001). Assessment of children: Cognitive applications . Jerome Sattler Publisher Inc.

Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C. P., Dulcan, M. K., & Schwab-Stone, M. E. (2000). NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children version IV (NIMH DISC- IV): Description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39 (1), 28–38. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200001000-00014

Sibley, M. H., Altszuler, A. R., Morrow, A. S., & Merrill, B. M. (2014). Mapping the academic problem behaviors of adolescents with ADHD. School Psychology Quarterly, 29 , 422–437.

Sonuga-Barke, E., Bitsakou, P., & Thompson, M. (2010). Beyond the dual pathway model: Evidence for the dissociation of timing, inhibitory, and delay-related impairments in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49 (4), 345–355. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-201004000-00009

Market research and market information group Kantar TNS the Netherlands, formarly known as TNS-NIPO (Vol. 2016).

Wåhlstedt, C., & Bohlin, G. (2010). DSM-IV-defined inattention and sluggish cognitive tempo: Independent and interactive relations to neuropsychological factors and comorbidity. Child Neuropsychology, 16 (4), 350–365.

Weller, E. B., Weller, R. A., Fristad, M. A., Rooney, M. T., & Schecter, J. (2000). Children’s interview for psychiatric syndromes (ChIPS). Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39 (1), 76–84.

Download references

Acknowledgements

Marieke de Vries and the data collection for the Community Sample were supported by Research Priority Area Yield ( https://yield.uva.nl/ ). Funding for the US samples was provided by a grant awarded to Steve W. Evans from the National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH082864) and the Institute for Educational Sciences (R305A140356). The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the funding agencies. Funding for the Dutch sample was provided by the Dutch Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMW) grant (#15700095007).

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

School of Psychology, Faculty of Science, University of Nottingham Malaysia, Jalan Broga, 43500, Semenyih, Malaysia

Marieke de Vries

Clinical Psychology, Psychology and Educational Sciences, Louvain, KU, Belgium

Saskia van der Oord

Leuven Brain Institute, Louvain, Belgium

Center for Intervention Research in Schools, Ohio University, Athens, OH, USA

Steven W. Evans

Lehigh University, Bethlehem, USA

George J. DuPaul

Clinical Developmental Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands

Bianca E. Boyer

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marieke de Vries .

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest.

We have no known conflicts of interest for this study.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

de Vries, M., van der Oord, S., Evans, S.W. et al. The Homework Problems Checklist: Psychometric Properties and Usefulness in teens with and without ADHD. School Mental Health 15 , 260–271 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12310-022-09548-9

Download citation

Accepted : 30 September 2022

Published : 28 October 2022

Issue Date : March 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s12310-022-09548-9

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • ADHD Homework Problems Psychometric Properties

Advertisement

  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us

Homework Problem Checklist (HPC)

Anesko‚ K. M.‚ Schoiock‚ G.‚ Rafael‚ L.‚ Fredric‚ M. (1987). The Homework Problem Checklist : Assessing children’s homework difficulties. Behavioral assessment‚ 9(2)‚ 179-185 .
Foley‚ R. M.‚ & Epstein‚ M. (1993). Evaluation of the homework problem checklist with students with behavior disorders. Special Services in the Schools‚ 7(1)‚ 79-90 .
Power‚ Thomas J.‚ Werba. Branlyn E.‚ Watkins‚ Marley W. et al. (2006). Patterns of Parent-Reported Homework Problems Among ADHD-Referred and Non-Referred Children. School Psychology Quarterly‚ 21(1)‚ 13-33.
Anesko‚ K. M.‚ Schoiock‚ G.‚ Rafael‚ L.‚ Fredric‚ M. (1987). Homework Problem Checklist (HPC). In Fischer‚ Joel.‚ Corcoran‚ Kevin J. (2007). Measures for Clinical Practice and research: A sourcebook. (4th ed.). NY. Oxford University Pr. Vol. 1‚ Page (s): 544-545.

Related terms:

  • additional module homework – Münster questionnaire for evaluation (MFE-ZHa)
  • Stalking Behavior Checklist
  • Life Events Checklist (Modified)
  • Problem checklist
  • Generative Behavior Checklist (GBC)
  • Checklist for Autism in Toddlers- CHAT
  • Pediatric Symptom Checklist (PSC-35)
  • Adult ADHD Self Report Scale (ASRS) v1.1. Symptom Checklist
  • Burns Depression Checklist (BDC)
  • FATIGUE CHECKLIST

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts

Logo of nihpa

Assessing Homework Problems in Children with ADHD: Validation of a Parent-Report Measure and Evaluation of Homework Performance Patterns

Joshua m. langberg.

Department of Pediatrics University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center

L. Eugene Arnold

Department of Psychiatry, Ohio State University

Amanda M. Flowers

Mekibib altaye, jeff n. epstein, brooke s.g. molina.

Departments of Psychiatry & Psychology, University of Pittsburgh

The factor structure of a parent-report measure of child homework problems, the Homework Problems Checklist, was examined in a geographically and ethnically diverse sample of children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). This measure was completed by the parents of 579 children ages 7.0-9.9 diagnosed with ADHD Combined Type as part of the Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (MTA). Results replicated previous work showing two salient factors that measure homework completion behaviors (Factor I) and homework management behaviors (Factor II). This two-factor solution remained consistent when examined across child sex and ethnicity subgroups. Analysis of patterns revealed that homework problems are greater for children in higher grades and that children with ADHD and comorbid Learning Disabilities experience significantly more homework problems than children with ADHD alone. This study also replicated previous work showing that homework problems and ADHD inattentive symptoms are highly correlated whereas correlations between homework problems and hyperactivity and impulsivity are low to moderate. Implications of the findings for the assessment of homework problems in children with ADHD and for intervention are discussed.

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is the most common neurobehavioral disorder in children with prevalence rates ranging from 3% to 10% ( Brown et al., 2001 ; Froehlich et al., 2007 ). Children with ADHD experience significant impairment across multiple domains of functioning and throughout the developmental lifespan ( Barkley, Fischer, Smallish, & Fletcher, 2002 ; Biederman, 2005 ). Poor academic achievement is arguably the most serious difficulty faced by children who meet criteria for ADHD ( DuPaul & Stoner, 2003 ; Massetti et al., 2008 ). Compared to their peers, children with ADHD have significantly lower standardized achievement test scores and school grades ( Frazier, Youngstrom, Glutting, & Watkins, 2007 ) and experience higher rates of academic failure and school dropout ( Barkley, Fischer, Edelbrock, & Smallish, 1990 ; DuPaul & Stoner, 2003 ; Epstein, Polloway, Foley, & Patton, 1993 ). In fact, although ADHD symptoms decline with increased chronological age ( Biederman, Mick, & Faraone, 2000 & Hart, Lahey, Loeber, Applegate, & Frick, 1995 ), academic impairments persist and may increase as children progress through school ( Massetti et al., 2008 ; Wolraich et al., 2005 ).

Homework and Academic Achievement

Difficulties with homework management and completion contribute to the academic problems experienced by children with ADHD. Children with ADHD have significantly more homework difficulties than their classroom peers ( Epstein et al., 1993 ; Lahey et al., 1994 ; Power, Werba, Watkins, Angelucci, & Eiraldi, 2006 ). Children with ADHD are more likely than their peers to forget to bring materials from school to home and vice versa, to have homework assignments recorded inaccurately, to procrastinate when completing homework assignments, and to have left work incomplete ( Evans et al., 2009 ; Langberg, Epstein, Urbanowicz, Simon, & Graham, 2008 ; Power et al., 2006 ). Children with ADHD often have disorganized school binders, bookbags, lockers, and desks and as a result, frequently lose and cannot find homework materials ( Evans et al., 2009 ; Langberg et al., 2008 ; Zentall, Harper & Stormont-Spurgin, 1993 ). Further, when completing homework, children with ADHD often have difficulties staying on-task, rush through their assignments and make careless mistakes ( Epstein et al., 1993 ; Power, Karustis, & Habboushe, 2001 ).

In the United States, homework completion is a major component of the educational curriculum ( West Chester Institute for Human Services Research, 2002 ) and is positively correlated with school grades and achievement test scores ( Cooper, Lindsay, Nye, & Greathouse, 1998 ; Cooper, 1989 ). The relationship between homework and academic achievement is moderated by grade in school and is stronger in secondary school (i.e. grades 7-12) than in elementary school ( Cooper, Robinson, & Patall, 2006 ). Homework also serves to facilitate family involvement in education ( Olympia, Sheridan, Jenson, & Andrews, 1994 ) which is a strong predictor of children’s academic achievement ( Fantuzzo, McWayne, Perry, & Childs, 2004 ). Accordingly, the homework difficulties that children with ADHD experience are an important area for research and intervention.

Measuring Homework Problems

Children with ADHD often experience difficulties with at least one homework-related behavior that ultimately results in poor homework performance ( Power et al., 2006 ). For example, a child may record homework assignments inaccurately or not at all, mismanage materials, be off-task during homework completion, or have a combination of these difficulties. A reliable and valid measure of homework performance that identifies specific areas of deficit and thoroughly assesses homework behavior is necessary to inform intervention strategy. The Homework Problem Checklist (HPC) is a commonly used instrument for assessing children’s homework performance ( Anesko, Schoiock, Ramirez, & Levine, 1987 ). Several studies support this 20-item, parent-report measure as an adequate screening and outcome tool that encompasses a variety of behaviors that are integral to successful completion of homework ( Anesko et al., 1987 ; Epstein et al., 1993 ; Lahey et al., 1994 ).

Until recently, the HPC was treated as a single factor instrument that broadly assessed the construct of homework performance. To evaluate the accuracy of this assumption, Power et al. (2006) examined the factor structure of the HPC in a sample of general education students ( N = 675) and in a clinic-based sample ( N = 356). Seventy-one percent of the children in the clinic-based sample met diagnostic criteria for ADHD according to the parent completed Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents (DICA-R-P; Reich, Shayka, & Taibleson, 1995 ). Exploratory factor analysis suggested that the HPC measures two distinct aspects of homework performance. These factors were extracted both in the general education sample and the clinic sample ( Power et al., 2006 ).

Factor I relates to problems that occur during homework completion. For example, parents rate their child’s efficiency of work completion, distractibility, inattention, and the parent-child interactions that occur during homework completion. Factor II relates predominately to homework management behaviors. For example, parents rate their child’s consistency in recording homework and in bringing home the necessary school materials. Both HPC factors have moderate to high correlations with the Inattention subscale of the Behavior Assessment Scale for Children (BASC; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992 ) parent version and low to moderate correlations with the Hyperactivity subscale ( Power et al., 2006 ).

The Power et al. (2006) factor analysis findings have not been replicated. A replication study is important because of a number of limitations related to the geographic and ethnic diversity of the sample. First, the ADHD sample was from a single clinic in the Northeast, significantly limiting geographic diversity. Second, the ethnic diversity of the sample was relatively limited (African American 13% and Latino 3%). Third, while Power et al. (2006) found that boys with ADHD had significantly more homework problems than girls, the sample had an insufficient number of girls to test the stability of the factor structure across sex. Finally, Intelligence Quotient (IQ) and Standardized Achievement Test Score data were not available and the contribution of Learning Disabilities to the identified homework problems could not be evaluated.

The primary purpose of this study is to explore patterns of parent-reported homework problems in a geographically and ethnically diverse sample of children with ADHD. The NIMH Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (MTA) sample examined in this study is geographically (six sites) and ethnically (38% minority) diverse and allows for evaluation of parent-reported homework problems across ethnic subgroups. A secondary goal of this study is to evaluate factors other than ethnicity that may be associated with homework problems in children with ADHD. There is some evidence to suggest that higher homework problem ratings (more severe problems) are associated with ADHD symptoms of inattention ( Power et al., 2006 ), grade in school ( Power et al., 2006 ) and male sex ( Anesko et al., 1987 ; Power et al., 2006 ). Accordingly, this study will examine the relationship between parent-rated homework problems and child sex, grade in school, and parent- and teacher-rated symptoms of ADHD. Further, while it has been demonstrated that children with learning problems ( Epstein et al., 1993 ) and children with ADHD ( Lahey et al., 1994 ) experience more homework problems than their peers, the impact of ADHD/LD comorbidity has not been examined. This study will test the hypothesis that an additive effect exists (i.e., that children with ADHD/LD comorbidity will exhibit significantly more homework problems than children with ADHD alone after accounting for intelligence).

Participants

The sample for this study is from the NIMH Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD (MTA; MTA Cooperative Group, 1999 ). Detailed descriptions of the MTA’s background and rationale, recruitment procedures, assessment and treatment methods, hypotheses, and study design have been reported in other publications ( Arnold et al., 1997 ; Hinshaw et al., 1997 ; MTA Cooperative Group, 1999 ). The MTA sample is geographically and ethnically diverse. The 579 participants were recruited from six separate sites across the United States and Canada. Sixty-two percent of the sample is Caucasian, 23% is African-American, 6% is Latino and 9% is of mixed decent or other ethnicity. The sample is also relatively socioeconomically diverse with 21% of the sample reporting a yearly family income below $20,000, 19% of the receiving welfare, and 23% of caregivers with a high school education or less. Eighty percent of MTA participants are male and 20% are female. All participants were between the ages of 7 and 9.9 at study entry (1 st through 4 th grades) and were diagnosed at baseline with ADHD, Combined Type using Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4 th edition ( DSM-IV ) criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). All parents/children signed informed consent/assent forms approved by the local Institutional Review Boards (IRB). As part of a comprehensive assessment battery completed at baseline, participants’ parents completed the HPC.

Homework Problem Checklist (HPC; Anesko et al., 1987 ). The HPC includes 20 items that parents rate regarding their child’s homework-related behavior. Parents are asked to rate the frequency with which these behaviors occur on 4-point Likert scales ranging from “never” to “very often.” Research has shown that the HPC measure has excellent internal consistency for children in 2 nd through 4 th grades, with alpha coefficients ranging from .90 to .92 and corrected item-total correlations ranging from .31 to .72 ( Anesko et al., 1987 ). HPC ratings completed by parents at baseline in the MTA were examined in this study.

SNAP-IV ( Swanson, 1992 ). ADHD and Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) symptoms were measured using the SNAP-IV Rating Scale. The SNAP includes the 18 ADHD items (9 DSM inattention and 9 DSM hyperactive/impulsive symptoms) and 8 ODD items from the DSM-IV. Parents and teachers respond on a 4-point Likert scale rating the severity of symptoms (i.e., 0 = not at all, 1 = just a little, 2 = pretty much, and 3 = very much). The scale yields ADHD-related factor scores for Inattention, Hyperactivity and Impulsivity and an ODD factor score. Each factor score is derived by summing the items for each symptom domain and dividing by the number of items on each factor (Inattention = 9 items; Hyperactivity = 6 items; Impulsivity = 3 items). Normative data for the SNAP are provided by Swanson (1992) . The 18 DSM ADHD items on the SNAP parent version were found to have excellent internal consistency in the MTA sample (Cronbach’s alpha = .97).

Statistical Analyses

Given the substantial differences in the Power et al. (2006) sample and the MTA sample (e.g., six sites across the U.S. versus a single site sample) and the fact that the Power et al. factor analysis findings have never been replicated, exploratory factor analysis was selected. As the primary goal of the study was to examine the factor structure reported by Power et al. (2006) , the factor analytic statistical procedures utilized by Power et al. were replicated. We utilized common factor analysis as opposed to principal component analysis because we were interested in the underlying latent structure of the HPC. The number of factors to be retained was determined by using a combination of criteria, including visual Scree plot (Keiser criterion; Eigenvalue > 1), MAP (Minimum Average Partials; Velicer, Eaton, & Fava, 2000 ) and parallel analysis ( O’Connor, 2000 ). Additionally, we looked at sampling adequacy as measured by the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) statistic. The KMO predicts, based on correlation and partial correlation data, whether items are likely to load on distinct factors adequately. The values range from 0 to 1 and with 0.6 or higher serving as a cut-point for proceeding with factor analysis (KMO; Kaiser, 1974 ). Anticipating correlated factors, we used oblique rotations and different rotation methods (varimax, equimax and promax) to identify the most interpretable factor structure. Salient factor loadings were defined as those whose values were greater than .40 ( Stevens, 2002 ). In addition, at least three salient item loadings were required to construct a factor ( Stevens, 2002 ). To remain consistent with Power et al. (2006) , we used a congruence coefficient (CC) to investigate the similarity of factor structures across racial/ethnic subgroups and sex. Congruence coefficients range in value from 0 to 1 with values of .85 −.94 corresponding to fair similarity across factors and .95 and above indicating the factor structure is virtually identical/equal ( Lorenzo-Seva & Berge, 2006 ).

The overall KMO ( Kaiser, 1974 ) statistic for all of the HPC items was .93 and ranged from .89 − .95 across the individual items. Examination of the correlation matrix revealed that most correlations were greater than .30. Initially, based on the Keiser criteria, a 3-factor solution was extracted. However, both the MAP and parallel analysis indicated that only two factors were needed. A two factor solution was also supported because only two variables (items 16 and 17) loaded on the third factor suggested by the Keiser criteria. Therefore, the third factor was eliminated and all further analyses using a variety of different rotation methods produced a 2-factor solution. As with Power et al. (2006) , Principal Factor extraction was used for all subsequent analyses followed by Promax rotation.

The two factors accounted for 50% of the variance. Twelve items loaded on Factor I (Cronbach’s alpha = .92) and seven items loaded on Factor II (Cronbach’s alpha = .86). The correlation between the two factors was .66 ( p <.0001). There were no cross-loadings (i.e., loadings greater than .40 on two factors), but items 4, 17, and 18 did not load well on either factor (see Table 1 ). Factor loadings for item 4 (.392 on Factor II) and 17 (.392 on Factor I) approached .4 in this study (see Table 1 ) and reached the .4 cutoff in the Power et al. (2006) general education sample. Item 18 did not load well on either factor in this study or in the Power et al. (2006) study. Accordingly, from this point forward, item 4 was included in calculating the Factor II score, item 17 in calculating the Factor I score, and item 18 was only included when calculating the HPC Total Score. We calculated the HPC Total Score and presented means and standard deviations in the tables primarily to allow comparisons with previous research.

Pattern Coefficients and Communalities Using Principal Axis Extraction and Promax Rotation

Note : Boldface indicates salient pattern coefficient (≥.40). Italics indicate items that approached the significant loading cutoff (.40) and that met the .4 threshold on these factors in the general education sample of the Power et al. (2006) study.

Ethnic Differences

The two factor structure of the HPC was examined across racial and ethnic subgroups (Caucasian N = 344; African American N = 109; Latino N = 40; Other N = 70). When compared across ethnicity, the CCs ranged from .95 − .97 for HPC Factor I and from .90 to .98 for HPC Factor II indicating that the two factor structure fit similarly across ethnicity. An ANOVA testing for differences in the severity of homework problems by each category of ethnicity was not significant ( p = .17; see Table 2 ).

Comparison of Means and Standard Deviations of Parent-Rated Homework Problems by Ethnic Subgroup

Note: ANOVA was not significant across subgroups; p = 0.17

Sex and Grade Differences

The sample was divided into male ( N = 464) and female ( N = 115). A two factor solution was generated and similarity of factor structure across sex was measured using a CC. The CC was .99 for HPC Factor I and .96 for HPC Factor II between the two samples (male and female), indicating that the two factor structures were virtually identical. Males were rated as exhibiting more severe homework problems than females for HPC Factor II ( p <.05) but not for HPC Factor I ( p = .07). Cohen’s d effect size calculations revealed that the differences between males and females on homework problem ratings were small (Factor II Male M = 8.68 & Female M = 7.57, d = .22; Factor I Male M = 24.47 & Female M = 22.92, d = .19).

The sample was next divided by grade in school (1 st grade N = 93; 2 nd grade N = 230; 3 rd grade N = 170; 4 th grade N = 70). An ANOVA conducted using the HPC Factors I and II as the dependent variables revealed a significant effect of grade ( p <.01). Homework problems ratings were highest (i.e. most problems) in grade 4 and lowest in grade 1 (see Table 3 ). Pairwise comparisons revealed that participants in grades 3 and 4 had significantly more homework problems than children in grades 1 and 2 ( p <.05) for both HPC Factor I and Factor II. There was no significant difference between children in grades 1 and 2 or between children in grades 3 and 4. Cohen’s d effect size analyses revealed that the difference in homework problems between children in grade 1 and grade 4 was moderate (Factor I d = .37; Factor II d = .47).

Comparison of Means and Standard Deviations of Parent-Rated Homework Problems by Grade in School

Note: Grades 4 & 3 > Grades 1 & 2; p <.05; Total Score = Sum of items; Item mean = Average item score

Correlations between HPC Factors and ADHD/ODD Symptoms

For these correlational analyses, the SNAP Inattention, Hyperactivity, Impulsivity, and ODD scores were each separately correlated with the two HPC factors. Similar to Power et al., (2006) , both HPC Factors had moderate to high correlations with parent ratings of inattention and low to moderate correlations with parent ratings of impulsivity and hyperactivity (all ps <.0001; see Table 4 ). The correlations between HPC Factors I and II and teacher ratings of inattention were lower but significant ( p <.001). Teacher ratings of hyperactivity and impulsivity were not correlated with parent ratings of homework problems (see Table 3 ). Also similar to Power et al. (2006) , both HPC Factors had moderate correlations with parent ratings of ODD symptoms ( p <.0001). Teacher ratings of ODD had small correlations with HPC Factor II ( p <.01) and were not significantly correlated with HPC Factor I.

Correlation between SNAP and HPC Factor Scores

ADHD/LD Comorbidity

Current best-practice recommendations for diagnosing a LD include documentation of an academic skills deficit as measured by a norm-referenced academic achievement test. An academic skills deficit is defined as a score of more than one standard deviation below the mean (a standardized score of 85 or below on most norm-referenced achievement tests; Dombrowski, Kamphaus, & Reynolds, 2004 ). All participants in the MTA were administered the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT; Wechsler, 1992 ) Reading, Math, and Spelling subtests at baseline. When LD is diagnosed on the basis of a score at or below 85 on one or more of these subtests, about one-third of the children in the MTA sample are identified ( Swanson et al., 2000 ), which is consistent with prevalence rates of ADHD/LD comorbidity ( DuPaul & Stoner, 2003 ). Dombrowski et al. (2004) specified a number of additional criteria that should be assessed as part of a comprehensive LD evaluation, including educational impairment and alternative explanations (e.g. cultural or economic factors). As children in this sample were diagnosed solely upon the < 85 criterion, they should be considered potential LD, rather than as meeting full diagnostic criteria for a LD. Using this definition, 192 participants (33% of sample) met criteria for at least one of the three types of LD. The male to female ratio for the LD sample mirrored the overall MTA sample (19% female). Sixty-five participants met criteria for two different types of LD and 42 participants met for all three types. For the analyses, participants who met for more than one type of LD were included in each group that they met criteria. Overall, N = 108 students met criteria for a potential Reading Disability (RD), N = 95 students for potential Math (MD), and N = 128 students for potential Spelling (SD).

An ANCOVA was conducted in order to control for participants’ Full Scale IQ as assessed by the WISC-III ( Wechsler, 1991 ). Children with ADHD/RD and ADHD/SD had significantly more homework problems than children with ADHD alone on both HPC Factors I and II after controlling for Full Scale IQ ( p <.05). The difference between parent-ratings of homework problems in children with ADHD alone in comparison to children with ADHD/RD (Factor I d = .29; Factor II d = .34) and ADHD/SD (Factor I d = .26; Factor II d = .28) was small. Children with ADHD/MD did not have more homework problems than children with ADHD alone for HPC Factor I ( p =.25) but did for HPC Factor II ( p <.05; d = .20; see Table 5 ). ANCOVA’s were also conducted using the HPC Total Score for comparison with prior research. Children with comorbid ADHD/RD ( d = .34) and ADHD/SD ( d = .30) were rated as having significantly more homework problems as measured by the HPC Total Score than children with ADHD alone after controlling for Full Scale IQ ( p <.01). There was no significant difference in parent-ratings of homework problems for children with ADHD alone as compared to children with ADHD/MD.

Comparison of Means and Standard Deviations of Parent-Rated Homework Problems by ADHD/LD Status

Note: RD = potential Reading Disability; MD = potential Math Disability; SD = potential Spelling Disability; ADHD/RD & ADHD/SD > ADHD Alone for HPC Factors I & II and Total Score ( p <.05); ADHD/MD > ADHD Alone only for HPC Factor II ( p <.05)

The results of an exploratory factor analysis with a geographically and ethnically diverse sample of elementary school-aged students with ADHD support the findings from Power et al. (2006) : the items of the HPC are best described by two distinct factors that measure homework completion behaviors and homework management behaviors. When examined across race and ethnic subgroups and across sex, the HPC two-factor solution was virtually identical. The similarity in findings between this study and the Power et al. (2006) study are remarkable in light of the significant differences between the two samples in relation to both sample diversity (MTA = six sites across U.S. & Power et al. = one clinic in Northeast; MTA = 38% minority & Power et al. = 19% minority) and participant diagnosis (MTA sample = 100% ADHD Combined Type & Power et al. = 75% ADHD with 28% Combined Type).

In both the present study and the Power et al. (2006) study, item 18 did not load well on either factor and items 4 and 17 had marginal loadings around .4. Item 4 “refuses to do homework assignments” loaded best on Factor II in both our study and in the Power et al. study. The loading of item 17 “hurries and makes careless mistakes” was sample dependent (i.e. general education or clinic) in the Power et al. (2006) study. Item 17 loaded on Factor I in the present study and in the Power et al. general education sample. Further, the item fits best conceptually with Factor I as the behavior “hurries and makes careless mistakes” occurs during the process of homework completion. Accordingly, future studies with the HPC should include item 4 in calculating the Factor II score and item 17 when calculating the Factor I score. Given the low loadings for item 18 (“dissatisfied with work, even when does a good job”) in both studies (the only item on HPC below .3 in both studies), it should not be included when calculating either factor and could either be: 1) dropped from the measure; 2) reworded; or 3) included only in calculating the HPC Total Score. Given that a substantial amount of previous research has included item 18 in calculating the HPC Total Score (e.g. Lahey et al., 1994 ; Langberg et al., 2008 ; Power et al., 2006 ), our recommendation is for future studies to continue including item 18 in calculating the HPC Total Score. This strategy should aid in interpretation of findings across studies.

Parent ratings of African American and Latino children did not differ from parent ratings of Caucasian children in homework problem severity (see Table 2 ) and the difference between boys and girls was small to negligible. Further, the two factor structure of the HPC was virtually identical when examined across ethnic subgroups and child sex. The lack of differences across ethnic subgroups and sex could be a function of the MTA sample including only children with ADHD Combined Type. Specifically, variability in homework problems was likely reduced because all children in the sample were referred for ADHD and associated impairments, which typically include poor school performance. Previous research has shown that the African American/Caucasian achievement gap is mediated by higher rates of attention difficulties among African Americans ( Rabiner, Murray, Schmid, & Malone, 2004 ). In fact, Rabiner et al. (2004) found that almost half of teacher-rated achievement differences were explained by the presence of attention problems. Accordingly, potential variability in homework problems across ethnic subgroups was likely limited because all children in MTA, by definition, had high rates of attention problems. This hypothesis is further supported by the fact that across studies with the HPC, the difference between boys and girls is larger in general education samples than in samples of children with attention problems ( Anesko et al., 1987 & Power et al., 2006 ).

Anesko et al. (1987) and Power et al. (2006) reported negligible differences in ratings of homework problem severity as a function of grade in school. In contrast, we found that homework problems increased significantly as a function of grade in school and that the difference in homework problems between the 1 st and 4 th grades was moderate (Factor I d = .37; Factor II d = .47; see Table 3 ). One possible explanation for the discrepancy is that Power et al. (2006) only examined the impact of grade in the general education sample and not in the sample of children with ADHD and the Anesko et al. (1987) sample was general education. It may be that the pattern of increasing homework problems with grade in school is only evident for children with learning and behavior problems such as ADHD.

As children progress through school numerous environmental changes occur, including increased demands for independence and greater academic workloads ( Evans, Langberg, Raggi, Allen & Buvinger, 2005 ; Langberg et al., 2008 ). In particular, more homework is assigned in higher grades and students spend greater amounts of time completing homework ( Campbell et al., 1996 ). The relationship between higher grade in school and increased homework problems may be a function of a deficit x environment interaction, an interaction that does not occur for children without certain deficits or difficulties. That is, it may become steadily more difficult for children with ADHD to compensate for certain deficits (e.g. difficulties with focus and materials management) and to be successful with homework as academic expectations increase. Children without these difficulties may be better able to adjust and may even become more adept with practice and challenge. After all, the homework and academic challenges that increase with grade level are designed to promote learning for the average child. It is interesting to note that the relationship between homework and academic achievement gets stronger as children progress through school and is strongest in secondary school ( Cooper et al., 2006 ). This may be partially explained by the fact that certain subsets of children experience steady increases in homework problems which subsequently impacts academic achievement to a greater extent.

Similar to Power et al. (2006) , we found that both HPC factors were highly correlated with parent ratings of inattention and that correlations with parent ratings of hyperactivity and impulsivity and teacher ratings of inattention were low to moderate (see Table 3 ). Lahey et al. (1994) reported similar findings as part of the DSM-IV field trials for children with ADHD. Specifically, symptoms of inattention predicted the parent-completed HPC but symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity did not. Further, children with Combined Type and Inattentive Type had significantly more homework problems than children with Hyperactive/Impulsive Type ( Lahey et al., 1994) . This finding has also been replicated with other indices of academic functioning, including grade point average ( Molina, Smith & Pelham, 2001 ) and standardized achievement test scores ( Massetti et al., 2008 ; Molina et al., 2001 ). The strong relationship between academic functioning and ADHD symptoms of inattention may partially explain why the academic impairments of children with ADHD persist over time. Specifically, most ADHD symptom trajectory studies have found that while symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity decline during adolescence symptoms of inattention persist ( Biederman, Mick, & Faraone, 2000 ; Hart et al., 1995 ).

We found that children with ADHD and below average reading or spelling achievement test scores exhibited significantly more Factor I and Factor II homework problems than did children with ADHD alone (see Table 5 ). Children with below average math achievement test scores also had more homework problems on Factor II than did children with ADHD alone. ADHD and LD are highly comorbid with approximately 30% of children with ADHD also meeting criteria for LD ( DuPaul & Stoner, 2003 ). There is a growing body of evidence demonstrating that comorbid ADHD/LD is associated with increased functional impairment in a number of areas above and beyond what is typical of either disorder alone (e.g., Mayes et al., 2000 ; McNamara et al., 2005 ). This effect of additive functional impairment may have implications for homework interventions. Children with ADHD/LD will likely require a combination of direct instruction targeting academic skills and behavioral intervention targeting homework management and completion behaviors.

Despite high rates of comorbidity and increased risk for negative outcomes, almost no research has been published on the efficacy of psychosocial or pharmacological interventions for children with comorbid ADHD/LD. Children with a LD have deficits in core skills such as reading, math, and writing that may not be improved with medication ( MTA Cooperative Group, 1999 ) or with psychosocial interventions that target homework management and organization of materials (e.g. Langberg et al., 2008 ; Power et al., 2001 ). One of the few studies to evaluate the efficacy of stimulant medications for children with ADHD/LD found that 55% of children with comorbid ADHD/LD made significant improvements on methylphenidate as compared to 75% of children with ADHD alone ( Grizenko, Bhat, Schwartz, Ter-Stepanian, & Joober, 2006 ). These preliminary findings suggest that traditional ADHD interventions may not be as effective for children with ADHD/LD and that further intervention development research is needed.

Limitations

The HPC is a parent completed measure. Teachers can undoubtedly provide unique and valuable information as part of a homework assessment. For example, teachers may be able to more accurately rate a child’s consistency with recording homework assignments, bringing homework assignments to class, and keeping homework materials organized in a locker or desk. Another limitation is that some of the HPC items overlap with symptoms of ADHD making it hard to measure the constructs independently. Recently, a teacher-report measure of homework problems was developed ( Power, Dombrowski, Watkins, Mautone, & Eagle, 2007 ). This measure, the Homework Performance Questionnaire (HPQ), has both parent and teacher versions and items do not directly overlap with ADHD symptoms. Future research on homework problem assessment and/or intervention should seek to use a multi-informant approach.

We did not find differences in homework problems as a function of ethnicity. While African American and Latino children have historically experienced less academic success and lower academic proficiency when compared to Caucasian children, this is largely attributable to differences in socioeconomic backgrounds ( Tucker & Herman, 2002 ; NCES, 2001 ). With 77% of caregivers in the MTA sample having at least some college education, the sample may have lacked the SES diversity necessary to detect differences in homework problems.

Clinical Implications

This research confirms the Power et al. (2006) finding that homework performance is not a unitary construct. This finding has implications for interventions targeting homework problems. Factor I on the HPC relates to problems that occur during homework completion. For example, parents rate their child’s efficiency of work completion, distractibility, inattention, and the parent-child interactions that occur during homework completion. For children with high scores on Factor I, behavioral interventions that teach parents techniques directly related to these problems are likely to be effective (e.g., Power et al., 2001 ). For example, parents should be taught strategies for structuring the homework environment (e.g. selecting a quiet location to minimize distractions), providing effective instructions, and setting up reward systems to encourage on-task behavior. It is evident from numerous studies that medication produces marked reductions in symptoms of inattention and distractibility. Accordingly, stimulant medication, and particularly a late afternoon dose, would likely produce marked improvements in the inattention and distractibility aspects of homework measured by Factor I.

Factor II on the HPC relates predominately to behaviors that take place outside of actual homework completion time. Most of the items relate to organization and management of homework materials (e.g. does not know what homework has been assigned, fails to bring home assignments, and forgets to bring assignments back to class). For children with high scores on Factor II, behavioral interventions that teach children and families materials organization and homework management skills are likely to be most effective (e.g., Evans, Langberg, Raggi, Allen & Buvinger, 2005 ; Langberg, Epstein, Altaye et al., 2008 ). Stimulant medication may serve to improve some aspects measured by HPC Factor II, but likely not all. For example, medication may improve forgetfulness, but does not teach children skills related to organizing their school materials, planning for tests/projects, accurately recording homework assignments and it does not improve parent-teacher communication. A recent study of the MTA treatments supports this assertion. Specifically, children with ADHD in a medication only group improved significantly on HPC Factor I relative to children in a community control but did improve on HPC Factor II relative to the community control ( Langberg et al., in press ).

In sum, the two-factor solution for the HPC has now been demonstrated in two separate samples. Clinicians are encouraged to utilize the HPC to assess students’ homework completion and homework management problems and to use the factor scores to determine the most appropriate avenues for intervention. Future research should be conducted on the predictive utility of this measure. Specifically, parent ratings of early childhood homework problems may be predictive of later academic underachievement. In particular, studies are needed that examine the relationship between parent-rated homework problems and grades in school.

Contributor Information

Joshua M. Langberg, Department of Pediatrics University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center.

L. Eugene Arnold, Department of Psychiatry, Ohio State University.

Amanda M. Flowers, Department of Psychiatry, Ohio State University.

Mekibib Altaye, Department of Pediatrics University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center.

Jeff N. Epstein, Department of Pediatrics University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center.

Brooke S.G. Molina, Departments of Psychiatry & Psychology, University of Pittsburgh.

  • American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th edition. American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC: 2000. DSM-IV. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Anesko KM, Schoiock G, Ramirez R, Levine FM. The Homework Problem Checklist: Assessing children’s homework problems. Behavioral Assessment. 1987; 9 :179–185. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Arnold LE, Abikoff HB, Cantwell DP, Conners CK, Elliott GR, Greenhill LL, et al. NIMH collaborative multimodal treatment study of children with ADHD (MTA): Design, methodology, and protocol evolution. Journal of Attention Disorders. 1997; 2 :141–158. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barkley RA, Fischer M, Edelbrock CS, Smallish L. The adolescent outcome of hyperactive children diagnosed by research criteria: I. An 8-year prospective follow-up study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1990; 29 :546–557. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Barkley RA, Fischer M, Smallish L, Fletcher K. The persistence of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder into your adulthood as a function of reporting source and definition of disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002; 111 :279–289. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Biederman J. Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A selective overview. Biological Psychiatry. 2005; 57 :1215–1220. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Biederman J, Mick E, Faraone SV. Age-dependent decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: impact of remission definition and symptom type. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000; 157 :816–818. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brown RT, Freeman WS, Perin JM, et al. Prevalence and assessment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in primary care settings. Pediatrics. 2001; 107 (3) Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/107/3/e43 . [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Campbell JR, Reese CM, O’Sullivan C, Dossey JA. NAEP 1994 Trends in Academic Progress. U.S. Department of Education; Washington, DC: 1996. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cooper H. Homework. Longman; White Plains, NY: 1989. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cooper H, Lindsay JJ, Nye B, Greathouse S. Relationships among attitudes about homework, amount of homework assigned and completed, and student achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1998; 90 :70–83. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cooper H, Robinson JC, Patall EA. Does homework improve academic achievement? A synthesis of research. Review of Educational Research. 2006; 76 (1):1–62. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dombrowski SC, Kamphaus RW, Reynolds CR. After the demise of the discrepancy: Proposed learning disabilities diagnostic criteria. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice. 2004; 35 :364–372. [ Google Scholar ]
  • DuPaul GJ, Stoner G. ADHD in the schools: Assessment and intervention strategies. 2nd ed. Guildford Press; New York, NY: 2003. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Epstein MH, Polloway EA, Foley RM, Patton JR. Homework: A comparison of teachers’ and parents’ perceptions of the problems experienced by students identified as having behavioral disorders, learning disabilities, or no disabilities. Remedial and Special Education. 1993; 14 :40–50. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Evans SW, Langberg J, Raggi V, Allen J, Buvinger E. Development of a school-based treatment program for middle school youth with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2005; 9 :343–353. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Evans SW, Schultz BK, White LC, Brady C, Sibley MH, Van Eck K. A school-based organization intervention for young adolescents with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. School Mental Health. 2009 DOI 10.1007/s12310-009-9009-6. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fantuzzo J, McWayne C, Perry MA, Childs S. Multiple dimensions of family involvement and their relations to behavioral and learning competencies for urban, low-income children. School Psychology Review. 2004; 33 :467–480. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Frazier TW, Youngstrom EA, Glutting JJ, Watkins MW. ADHD and achievement: Meta-analysis of the child, adolescent, and adult literatures and a concomitant study with college students. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2007; 40 (1):49–65. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Froehlich TE, Lanphear BP, Epstein JN, Barberesi WJ, Katusic SK, Kahn RS. Prevalence and treatment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in a national sample of U.S. children. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 2007; 161 (9):857–864. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grizenko N, Bhat M, Schwartz G, Ter-Stepanian M, Joober R. Efficacy of methylphenidate in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and learning disabilities: A randomized crossover trial. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience. 2006; 31 (1) [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hart E, Lahey B, Loeber R, Applegate B, Frick P. Developmental change in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in boys: a four-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1995; 23 :729–749. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hinshaw SP, March JS, Abikoff H, Arnold LE, Cantwell DP, Conners CK, et al. Comprehensive assessment of childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in the context of a multisite, multimodal clinical trial. Journal of Attention Disorders. 1997; 1 (4):217–234. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kaiser HF. An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika. 1974; 39 :31–36. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lahey BB, Applegate B, McBurnett K, Biederman J, Greenhill L, Hynd GW, et al. DSM-IV field trials for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1994; 151 (11):1673–1685. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Langberg JM, Arnold LE, Flowers AM, Epstein JN, Altaye M, Hinshaw SP, et al. Parent-reported homework problems in the MTA study: Evidence for sustained improvement with behavioral treatment. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. in press. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Langberg JM, Epstein JN, Altaye M, Molina B, Arnold E, Vitiello B. The transition to middle school is associated with changes in the developmental trajectory of ADHD symptomatology in young adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2008; 37 (3):651–663. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Langberg JM, Epstein JN, Urbanowicz C, Simon J, Graham A. Efficacy of an organization skills intervention to improve the academic functioning of students with ADHD. School Psychology Quarterly. 2008; 23 (3):407–417. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lorenzo-Seva U, Berge JMF. Tucker’s congruence coefficient as a meaningful index of factor similarity. Methodology. 2006; 2 (2):57–64. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Massetti GM, Lahey BB, Pelham WE, Loney J, Ehrhardt A, Lee SS, et al. Academic achievement over 8 years among children who met modified criteria for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder at 4-6 Years of Age. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008; 36 :399–410. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mayes SD, Calhoun SL, Crowell EW. Learning disabilities and ADHD: Overlapping spectrum disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2000; 33 (5):417–424. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McNamara JK, Willoughby T, Chalmers H, YLC-CURA Psychosocial status of adolescents with learning disabilities with and without comorbid attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice. 2005; 20 (4):234–244. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Molina BSG, Hinshaw SP, Swanson JM, Arnold LE, Vitiello B, Jensen PS, et al. The MTA at 8 years: Prospective follow-up of children treated for combined type ADHD in a multisite study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Molina BSG, Smith BH, Pelham WE. Factor structure and criterion validity of secondary school teacher ratings of ADHD and ODD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2001; 29 :71–82. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • MTA Cooperative Group A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999; 56 :1073–1086. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • O’Connor BP. SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of components using parallel analysis and Velicer’s MAP test. Behavior Research Methods, Instrumentation, and Computers. 2000; 32 :396–402. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Olympia DE, Sheridan SM, Jenson WR, Andrews D. Using student-managed interventions to increase homework completion and accuracy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1994; 27 :85–99. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Power TJ, Dombrowski SC, Watkins MW, Mautone JA, Eagle JW. Assessing children’s homework performance: Development of multi-dimensional, multi-informant rating scales. Journal of School Psychology. 2007; 45 :333–348. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Power TJ, Karustis JL, Habboushe DF. Homework Success for Children with ADHD: A Family-School Intervention Program. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2001. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Power TJ, Werba BE, Watkins MW, Angelucci JG, Eiraldi RB. Patterns of parent-reported homework problems among adhd-referred and non-referred children. School Psychology Quarterly. 2006; 21 :13–33. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rabiner DL, Murray DW, Schmid L, Malone PS. An exploration of the relationship between ethnicity, attention problems, and academic achievement. School Psychology Review. 2004; 33 (4):498–509. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reich W, Shayka MA, Taibleson C. Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents: DSM-IV Revision (Parent Form) Washington University; St Louis, MO: 1995. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reynolds CR, Kamphaus RW. Manual for the Behavior Assessment System for Children. American Guidance Service; Circle Pines, MN: 1992. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stevens JP. Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences. 4th Ed. Allyn and Bacon; Boston: 2002. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Swanson JM. School Based Assessments and Interventions for ADD Students. K.C.; Irvine, CA: 1992. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Swanson JM, Hanley T, Simpson S, Davies M, Schulte A, Wells K, et al. Evaluation of learning disorders in children with psychiatric disorders: An example from the Multimodal Treatment Study for ADHD. In: Greenhill LL, editor. Learning Disability. Implications for Psychiatric Treatment, Review of Psychiatry. Vol. 19. American Psychiatric Press, Inc.; Washington, DC: 2000. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tucker CM, Herman KC. Using culturally-sensitive theories and research to meet the academic needs of low-income African American children. American Psychologist. 2002; 57 :762–773. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • U.S. Department of Education. National Center for Education Statistics . The Condition of Education 2001. NCES 2001-072. U.S. Government Printing Office; Washington, DC: 2001. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Velicer WF, Eaton CA, Fava JL. Construct explication through factor or component analysis: A review and evaluation of alternative procedures for determining the number of factors or components. In: Goffin RD, Helmes E, editors. Problems and Solutions in Human Assessment. Kluwer; Boston: 2000. pp. 41–71. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 3rd ed. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1991. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wechsler . Wechsler Individual Achievement Test Manual. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1992. [ Google Scholar ]
  • West Chester Institute for Human Services Research The balanced view: Homework. 2002 June; Retrieved January 30, 2009 from http://www.sharingsuccess.org/code/bv/homework.pdf .
  • Wolraich ML, Wibbelsman CJ, Brown TE, Evans SW, Gotlieb EM, Knight JR, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among adolescents: A review of the diagnosis, treatment, and clinical implications. Pediatrics. 2005; 115 (6):1734–1746. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zentall SS, Harper GW, Stormont-Spurgin M. Children with hyperactivity and their organizational abilities. Journal of Educational Research. 1993; 87 (2):112–117. [ Google Scholar ]

Homework Checklist

  • education , children
  • By Laura Cooper Peterson on Dec 26, 2011
  • Create the Environment
  • Make sure children have a fairly quiet place to study with lots of light.
  • Put together a homework kit.
  • Check if children have access to the necessary resources.
  • Set a regular time every day for homework.
  • Find appropriate background music.
  • Show support by staying nearby, whenever possible.
  • Use positive reinforcement, when appropriate.
  • Check if children have time to unwind after school.
  • Check if children have a homework "buddy".
  • Offer a nutritious snack to children before homework is started.
  • Encourage children to take short breaks while doing homework.
  • Ask children not to distract each other while they are doing homework.
  • Show children that homework is important.
  • Resolve Problems
  • Stay in touch with children teachers.
  • Contact the teacher early in the year before any problems arise.
  • Cooperate with the teacher to work out a plan and a schedule to resolve homework problems.
  • Follow up with teachers and with children to make sure the plans are working.
  • Provide Guidance
  • Draw the line between providing guidance and doing the homework for your children.
  • Understand and respect your children's styles of learning.
  • Check if children work better alone or with someone else.
  • Encourage your children to develop good study habits.
  • Help your children to get organized.
  • Talk with your children about homework assignments.
  • Make sure children understand homework assignements.
  • Praise your children when it is appropriate.
  • Make sure you know the school's homework policy.
  • Make sure assignments are started and completed.
  • Read the teacher's comments on assignments that are returned.
  • Share via: Twitter , LinkedIn , Facebook , Whatsapp
  • 240 views today
  • 27 uses today

Related Checklists

Lawless French - Unit 1

By Lawless French

Lawless Spanish - Unit 1

By Lawless Spanish

Lawless French - Unit 2

More Related Checklist Templates

Lawless French - Unit 3

More Checklist Templates

"homework checklist" not suitable, search the world's largest free library of checklist templates.

IMAGES

  1. Having Problems with Homework? Check What Is a Way to Finish Homework

    homework problems checklist

  2. 15 Best Free Printable Homework Checklist

    homework problems checklist

  3. Daily Homework Checklist FREEBIE by Teaching State of Mind

    homework problems checklist

  4. 11 Homework Checklist ideas

    homework problems checklist

  5. Free Homework Checklist Printable

    homework problems checklist

  6. Homework Checklist Pdf

    homework problems checklist

VIDEO

  1. homework problems 😪

  2. Alg2: Mixed Factoring Homework Problems

  3. Quadratic Formula (homework problems)

  4. ACC 101 Chapter 6 Homework problems

  5. Day 28: Homework Example #4

  6. Day 28: Homework Example #1

COMMENTS

  1. How to Break Down and Solve Complex Math Problems in Your Homework

    Math homework can often be a challenging task, especially when faced with complex problems that seem daunting at first glance. However, with the right approach and problem-solving techniques, you can break down these problems into manageabl...

  2. Unlocking the Secrets of Math Homework: Expert Techniques to Solve Any Problem

    Math homework can sometimes feel like an insurmountable challenge. From complex equations to confusing word problems, it’s easy to get overwhelmed. However, with the right techniques and strategies, you can conquer any math problem that com...

  3. Network Connectivity Problems: A Step-by-Step Troubleshooting Checklist

    Network connectivity problems can be frustrating, especially when they disrupt your productivity or prevent you from accessing important resources. The first step in troubleshooting network connectivity problems is to ensure that all your h...

  4. The Homework Problems Checklist: Psychometric Properties and

    The Homework Problems Checklist (HPC) is a parent-rated questionnaire to assess homework problems that was studied with children but not

  5. Homework Problem Checklist (HPC)

    Anesko‚ K. M.‚ Schoiock‚ G.‚ Rafael‚ L.‚ Fredric‚ M. (1987). The Homework Problem Checklist: Assessing children's homework difficulties. Behavioral.

  6. The Homework Problem Checklist

    Describes the Homework Problem Checklist (HPC), an attempt to develop a measure of the frequency and intensity of children's homework problems.

  7. ERIC

    Homework problems are frequently encountered, especially among youth with ADHD. The Homework Problems Checklist (HPC) is a parent-rated questionnaire to

  8. Evaluation of the Homework Problem Checklist with Students with

    Anesko, K.M., Schoiock, G., Ramirez, R., & Levine, F.M. (1987). The Homework Problem Checklist: Assessing children's homework difficulties. Behavioral

  9. Evluation of the Homework Problem Checklist with Students with

    The Homework Problem Checklist (HPC) (Anesko et al., 1987) lists 20 statements about problems that may occur when a student does homework (see Table 1). The

  10. Homework Problem Checklist (HPC) for self-directed learning

    Download scientific diagram | Homework Problem Checklist (HPC) for self-directed learning. from publication: Effects of a Cognitive-Functional Intervention

  11. The Homework Problem Checklist: Assessing children's homework

    Semantic Scholar extracted view of "The Homework Problem Checklist: Assessing children's homework difficulties." by Kathleen M. Anesko et al.

  12. Assessing Homework Problems in Children with ADHD: Validation

    The Homework Problem Checklist (HPC) is a commonly used instrument for assessing children's homework performance (Anesko, Schoiock, Ramirez, & Levine, 1987).

  13. Homework Checklist (31 tasks) [PDF

    Contact the teacher early in the year before any problems arise. Cooperate with the teacher to work out a plan and a schedule to resolve homework problems.

  14. Homework Checklist Template for Students

    How do I help my child with homework? · Find a dedicated quiet space to work. · Make sure you have the right software and equipment to complete tasks. · Take